The Effects of School-based Physical Exercise With Different Cognitive Loads on Executive Functions

Document Type : Research Paper

Authors

Department of Motor Behavior, Faculty of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Allameh Tabataba’i University, Tehran, Iran.

Abstract

Introduction: The present study aimed to compare the effects of 3 school-based physical exercises with different levels of cognitive engagement on executive functions. 
Materials and Methods: In total, 47 students aged 13-14 years were randomly divided into 3 groups, as follows: the Integrated Physical and Cognitive (IPC) exercise, peer physical exercise group (with the same physical challenge), and control group (the routine physical education program). A Continuous Performance Test (CPT), the N-back test, and the Stroop test were used to evaluate executive functions in pretest and posttest. 
Results: The obtained results indicated that the IPC group significantly improved more than the other groups on the executive functions. Accordingly, physical exercise with a higher cognitive load benefitted both speed and accuracy on cognitive tasks significantly more than the same physical activity alone. 
Conclusion: Therefore, in the integrated physical and cognitive exercise, the cognitive and physiological effects of this training style interact with each other; therefore, their beneficial effects can be gradually increased, while interventions that only focus on the physical dimension would be less effective in promoting cognitive functions.

Keywords


  1. Hodges JR. Cognitive assessment for clinicians. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2017. https://books.google.com/books?id=QYc0DwAAQBAJ&dq
  2. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology. 2013; 64:135-68. [DOI:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750] [PMID] [PMCID]
  3. Luna B. Developmental changes in cognitive control through adolescence. Advances in Child Development and Behavior. 2009; 37:233-78. [DOI:10.1016/S0065-2407(09)03706-9] [PMID] [PMCID]
  4. Westfall DR, Gejl AK, Tarp J, Wedderkopp N, Kramer AF, Hillman CH, et al. Associations between aerobic fitness and cognitive control in adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018; 9:1298. [DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01298] [PMID] [PMCID]
  5. Diamond A. Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2012; 21(5):335-41. [DOI:10.1177/0963721412453722] [PMID] [PMCID]
  6. Diamond A, Ling DS. Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2016; 18:34-48. [DOI:10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005] [PMID] [PMCID]
  7. Diamond A, Ling DS. Review of the evidence on, and fundamental questions about, efforts to improve executive functions, including working memory. In: Novick JM, Michael F. Bunting, Dougherty MR, Engle RW. Cognitive and Working Memory Training: Perspectives From Psychology, Neuroscience, And Human Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2019. p. 143-431. [DOI:10.1093/oso/9780199974467.003.0008]
  8. Diamond A. Effects of physical exercise on executive functions: Going beyond simply moving to moving with thought. Annals of Sports Medicine and Research. 2015; 2(1):1011. [PMID] [PMCID]
  9. Diamond A, Ling DS. Aerobic-exercise and resistance-training interventions have been among the least effective ways to improve executive functions of any method tried thus far. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2019; 37:100572. [DOI:10.1016/j.dcn.2018.05.001] [PMID] [PMCID]
  10. Tarp J, Domazet SL, Froberg K, Hillman CH, Andersen LB, Bugge A. Effectiveness of a school-based physical activity intervention on cognitive performance in Danish adolescents: LCoMotion-learning, cognition and motion-a cluster randomized controlled trial. PloS One. 2016; 11(6):e0158087. [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0158087] [PMID] [PMCID]
  11. Schmidt M, Jäger K, Egger F, Roebers CM, Conzelmann A. Cognitively engaging chronic physical activity, but not aerobic exercise, affects executive functions in primary school children: A group-randomized controlled trial. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2015; 37(6):575-91. [DOI:10.1123/jsep.2015-0069] [PMID]
  12. Erickson KI, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. Physical activity, brain, and cognition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015; 4:27-32. [DOI:10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.005]
  13. Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: Contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Developmental Review. 2010; 30(4):331-51. [DOI:10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001] [PMID]
  14. Ahn S, Fedewa AL. A meta-analysis of the relationship between children’s physical activity and mental health. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2011; 36(4):385-97. [DOI:10.1093/jpepsy/jsq107] [PMID]
  15. Hillman CH, Castelli DM, Buck SM. Aerobic fitness and neurocognitive function in healthy preadolescent children. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2005; 37(11):1967-74. [DOI:10.1249/01.mss.0000176680.79702.ce] [PMID]
  16. Raine LB, Biggan JR, Baym CL, Saliba BJ, Cohen NJ, Hillman CH. Adolescent changes in aerobic fitness are related to changes in academic achievement. Pediatric Exercise Science. 2018; 30(1):106-14. [DOI:10.1123/pes.2015-0225] [PMID]
  17. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development. 2000; 71(1):44-56. [DOI:10.1111/1467-8624.00117] [PMID]
  18. Budde H, Voelcker-Rehage C, Pietraßyk-Kendziorra S, Ribeiro P, Tidow G. Acute coordinative exercise improves attentional performance in adolescents. Neuroscience Letters. 2008; 441(2):219-23. [DOI:10.1016/j.neulet.2008.06.024] [PMID]
  19. Rigoli D, Piek JP, Kane R, Oosterlaan J. An examination of the relationship between motor coordination and executive functions in adolescents. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2012; 54(11):1025-31. [DOI:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04403.x] [PMID]
  20. Haynes W, Waddington G, Adams RD. Associations among balance, physical activity, physical fitness, and academic results in schoolchildren. Health Behavior and Policy Review. 2018; 5(1):57-68. [DOI:10.14485/HBPR.5.1.6]
  21. Mihara M, Miyai I, Hatakenaka M, Kubota K, Sakoda S. Role of the prefrontal cortex in human balance control. Neuroimage. 2008; 43(2):329-36. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.029] [PMID]
  22. Liu JH, Alderman BL, Song TF, Chen FT, Hung TM, Chang YK. A randomized controlled trial of coordination exercise on cognitive function in obese adolescents. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2018; 34:29-38. [DOI:10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.09.003]
  23. Bittmann FN, Gutschow S, Luther S, Wessel N, Kurths J. On the functional relationship between postural motor balance and performance at school. Deutsche Zeitschrift für Sportmedizin. 2005; 56(10):348-52. https://publishup.uni-potsdam.de/frontdoor/index/index/docId/13501
  24. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008; 9(1):58-65. [DOI:10.1038/nrn2298] [PMID]
  25. Pesce C. Shifting the focus from quantitative to qualitative exercise characteristics in exercise and cognition research. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2012; 34(6):766-86. [DOI:10.1123/jsep.34.6.766] [PMID]
  26. Pesce C, Crova C, Marchetti R, Struzzolino I, Masci I, Vannozzi G, et al. Searching for cognitively optimal challenge point in physical activity for children with typical and atypical motor development. Mental Health and Physical Activity. 2013; 6(3):172-80. [DOI:10.1016/j.mhpa.2013.07.001]
  27. Tomporowski PD, McCullick B, Pendleton DM, Pesce C. Exercise and children’s cognition: The role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2015; 4(1):47-55. [DOI:10.1016/j.jshs.2014.09.003]
  28. Oberer N, Gashaj V, Roebers CM. Executive functions, visual-motor coordination, physical fitness and academic achievement: Longitudinal relations in typically developing children. Human Movement Science. 2018; 58:69-79. [DOI:10.1016/j.humov.2018.01.003] [PMID]
  29. Hillman CH, McAuley E, Erickson KI, Liu-Ambrose T, Kramer AF. On mindful and mindless physical activity and executive function: A response to Diamond and Ling (2016). Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2019; 37:100529. [DOI:10.1016/j.dcn.2018.01.006] [PMID] [PMCID]
  30. Luz C, Rodrigues LP, Cordovil R. The relationship between motor coordination and executive functions in 4th grade children. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2015; 12(2):129-41. [DOI:10.1080/17405629.2014.966073]
  31. Novick JM, Bunting MF, Dougherty MR, Engle RW. Cognitive and working memory training: Perspectives from psychology, neuroscience, and human development. Oxford Scholarship Online. 2020. [DOI:10.1093/oso/9780199974467.001.0001]
  32. Covey TJ, Shucard JL, Shucard DW. Working memory training and perceptual discrimination training impact overlapping and distinct neurocognitive processes: Evidence from event-related potentials and transfer of training gains. Cognition. 2019; 182:50-72. [DOI:10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.012] [PMID]
  33. Riccio CA, Reynolds CR, Lowe PA. Clinical applications of continuous performance tests: Measuring attention and impulsive responding in children and adults. Hoboken: Wiley; 2001. https://books.google.com/books?id=hq5rAAAAMAAJ&q
  34. León-Domínguez U, Martín-Rodríguez JF, León-Carrión J. Executive n-back tasks for the neuropsychological assessment of working memory. Behavioural Brain Research. 2015; 292:167-73. [DOI:10.1016/j.bbr.2015.06.002] [PMID]
  35. Gorfein DS, MacLeod CM, MacLeod CM. Inhibition in cognition. Washington DC: American Psychological Association; 2007. https://books.google.com/books?id=M19qAAAAMAAJ&q
  36. McMorris T, Sproule J, Turner A, Hale BJ. Acute, intermediate intensity exercise, and speed and accuracy in working memory tasks: A meta-analytical comparison of effects. Physiology & Behavior. 2011; 102(3-4):421-8. [DOI:10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.007] [PMID]
  37. Ward N, Paul E, Watson P, Cooke GE, Hillman CH, Cohen NJ, et al. Enhanced learning through multimodal training: Evidence from a comprehensive cognitive, physical fitness, and neuroscience intervention. Scientific Reports. 2017; 7(1):5808. [DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-06237-5] [PMID] [PMCID]
  38. Crova C, Struzzolino I, Marchetti R, Masci I, Vannozzi G, Forte R, et al. Cognitively challenging physical activity benefits executive function in overweight children. Journal of Sports Sciences. 2014; 32(3):201-11. [DOI:10.1080/02640414.2013.828849] [PMID]
  39. Northey JM, Cherbuin N, Pumpa KL, Smee DJ, Rattray B. Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: A systematic review with meta-analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine. 2018; 52(3):154-60. [DOI:10.1136/bjsports-2016-096587] [PMID]
  40. Drollette ES, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Scudder MR, Moore RD, Kao SC, et al. Effects of the FITKids physical activity randomized controlled trial on conflict monitoring in youth. Psychophysiology. 2018; 55(3):e13017. [DOI:10.1111/psyp.13017] [PMID] [PMCID]
  41. Moreau D, Morrison AB, Conway AR. An ecological approach to cognitive enhancement: Complex motor training. Acta Psychologica. 2015; 157:44-55. [DOI:10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.02.007] [PMID]
  42. Barbey AK, Koenigs M, Grafman J. Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex. 2013; 49(5):1195-205. [DOI:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022] [PMID] [PMCID]
  43. Davidson MC, Amso D, Anderson LC, Diamond A. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia. 2006; 44(11):2037-78. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006] [PMID] [PMCID]
  44. Kao S-C, Drollette ES, Ritondale JP, Khan N, Hillman CH. The acute effects of high-intensity interval training and moderate-intensity continuous exercise on declarative memory and inhibitory control. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2018; 38:90-9. [DOI:10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.05.011]